(upbeat music) - I think the main way to determine what experiments need to do is to write down what questions you're trying to answer. One you write down whatever type of question you're trying to answer if you look in the literature and see how would I answer this question, what experiments kind of mimic what I want to do, what I'm trying to get at? Once I do that, then I'm like oh this is definitely the next step. When I'm looking at mice, how do I design an orthotopic xenograph model? How do I look at breast cancer development in mice? How do I look at human breast cancer cells being colonized in the lung? How exactly do I look at that experiment? I think the main thing is reading the literature, trying to figure out what experiment I'm trying to do from the literature and seeing how does that fit into the context of what I'm studying. - When you're trying to figure out an experimental strategy it's worth while to remember that even though we think that there are a lot of different kinds of experiments, they actually fall into some pretty straight forward categories. In general if you can think about what are my pertubation tools, what are my measurements that I can make, what's the time scale at which I can make those measurements, and in how many individuals can I make those measurements? I think it can be really useful to break things down in that way. What are my perturbation tools? For instance, you can break the components and measure what happens, over express or augment the activity of the components and see what happens, look at natural populations and how they are different from one another in whatever feature you want to look at. Those things map on different systems in different ways. Ask yourself what are you going to measure? Are you going to measure a molecular phenotype or are you going to measure an organismal phenotype like behavior or a developmental character or something like that. Once you think about that, what's the timescale at which I can make those measurements? Are you going to measure it continuously over time in a single case or are you going to measure it statistically at one time over lots of individuals and what are the caveats of doing both of those things in those two ways. There's usually a trade off somewhere between the through put with which you can do something and the resolution with which you can do it. Then ask in how many individuals can I make those measurements? Be they individual cells or individual organisms. That's sort of how's the material processed such as am I looking at single events or am I looking at average cohort either over space and time. The cam help you whittle down what techniques are available to you in each one of those cases. - We have a classification system in my laboratory for three kinds of experiments and results. Class one experiments are those where either answer is interesting. They're actually relatively rare to find class one. Most of the experiments that we do are what we call class two where usually only the positive result is meaningful and the negative result doesn't actually move the ball. It's really important to know the distinction between class one and class two. If you get it wrong, you start chasing negative results that you think are meaningful but are not. You can really go off in the wrong direction. Class three experiments of course are those where neither answer is interesting and informative. They're to avoided at all costs. What would a class one experiment look like? One kind of experiment is to be descriptive. That challenge of then deciding which characteristics will we look at, how broadly will be catalog them, will it just in one cell type and so forth. Are all decisions that need to be made but you can see that the experiments are class one. It doesn't matter if there's a methylation on this histone at that position, you just want to know if it's there or not. And under what conditions is it there versus not there. It's all informative, it's all class one. It's really important in planning of every experiment to ask if this class one, two or three? - This is going to sound very self-critical, I wish I would have known the extend of my own laziness in terms of doing science because basically the way I approached grad school was using the tools that I've already been developed in the lab and applying them to specific projects. I wish I had been more open to considering new types of measurements and techniques earlier because the window of opportunity for incorporating new skills into your research repertoire shrinks over time as you try and zero in on a project and get it done in order to graduate. In not being adventurous, I missed out on the opportunity to do the right experiments at the right time. Looking at a given project, you can attack it from different angles all the time. You basically choose the angles that you think are best scientifically and experiments that you think you can perform. Those are the two variables that you kind of optimize. It's the second one that's the key. The ones that you think you can perform. If you are exposed to lots of different techniques early on, then you're not longer limited by that and can perform any experiment, especially the one that you absolutely need to further the project at a given time. I think that's really important to keep in mind. As you broaden yourself out early on, it will help you attack the right experimental questions as you continue in your project. - I think the most important part of picking and choosing from existing techniques or developing new ones is to understand what your investment in doing something new is going to get you. In this sense what you really want to do is kind of black box the technique. You want to think if I could do this thing and I'm going to black box it, I don't know how you do it at all. But if I could feed this thing into it and get this thing out of it, then I would be able to learn what? What would be the great value here? I'm a big believer in the fact that as scientists we should communicate openly with one another and share tools and resources. Because that maximizes our investment in science as a whole. There's no reason for us to duplicate efforts if we don't have to. In that sense I'm very reluctant to invest technically in methods that I think would be appropriate for us to use if somebody else is already doing them. - In making progress on a research question after your convinced that the system is working and you can make some good measurements, I think it's important to start writing papers in your head. Or at least thinking about what set of possible figures would constitute a paper that people would be excited to read. In doing that it points you to experiments and also holes in existing experiments that you can follow up on and fill over the course of your graduate career. I recommend it as a tool in terms of conceptualizing your project and its direction.